Show simple item record

dc.contributor.authorRomøren, Anna Sara Hexeberg
dc.contributor.authorChen, Aoju
dc.date.accessioned2023-02-27T10:04:15Z
dc.date.available2023-02-27T10:04:15Z
dc.date.created2021-01-29T11:21:12Z
dc.date.issued2021
dc.identifier.citationJournal of Child Language. 2021, 49 (2), 213-238.en_US
dc.identifier.issn0305-0009
dc.identifier.issn1469-7602
dc.identifier.urihttps://hdl.handle.net/11250/3054128
dc.description.abstractWe investigated how Central Swedish-speaking four-to-eleven year-old children acquire the prosodic marking of narrow focus, compared to adult controls. Three measurements were analysed: placement of the prominence-marking high tone (prominence H), pitch range effects of the prominence H, and word duration. Subject-verb-object sentences were elicited in sentence-medial and sentence-final focus conditions via a semi-spontaneous elicitation task. The children largely performed in an adult-like manner already at four to five: They predominantly add prominence H to focal words and avoid this tone post-focally in both sentence-medial and sentence-final position. The placement or avoidance of prominence H has largely the same effects on pitch range for children and adults. Finally, four to five-year-olds and seven to eight-year-olds also increase the duration of the focal word, similar to adults. Hence, Central Swedish-speaking children master the use of prosody for focus marking at an earlier age, compared to children acquiring a West Germanic language.en_US
dc.language.isoengen_US
dc.publisherCambridge University Pressen_US
dc.relation.ispartofseriesJournal of Child Language;Volume 49 , Issue 2
dc.rightsAttribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 Internasjonal*
dc.rights.urihttp://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/deed.no*
dc.titleThe acquisition of prosodic marking of narrow focus in Central Swedishen_US
dc.typePeer revieweden_US
dc.typeJournal articleen_US
dc.description.versionpublishedVersionen_US
dc.rights.holder© The Author(s), 2021en_US
cristin.ispublishedtrue
cristin.fulltextoriginal
cristin.qualitycode2
dc.identifier.doihttps://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000920000847
dc.identifier.cristin1882176
dc.source.journalJournal of Child Languageen_US
dc.source.volume49en_US
dc.source.issue2en_US
dc.source.pagenumber213-238en_US


Files in this item

Thumbnail

This item appears in the following Collection(s)

Show simple item record

Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 Internasjonal
Except where otherwise noted, this item's license is described as Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 Internasjonal